THE AMERICA ONE NEWS
Jun 2, 2025  |  
0
 | Remer,MN
Sponsor:  QWIKET 
Sponsor:  QWIKET 
Sponsor:  QWIKET: Elevate your fantasy game! Interactive Sports Knowledge.
Sponsor:  QWIKET: Elevate your fantasy game! Interactive Sports Knowledge and Reasoning Support for Fantasy Sports and Betting Enthusiasts.
back  
topic
The Economist
The Economist
15 Oct 2023


NextImg:How to predict the outcome of a coin toss
Science and technology | News you can use

How to predict the outcome of a coin toss

Coins are fair. Their tossers, less so

Legend holds that the city of Portland, Oregon, was very nearly called Boston. A coin toss held in 1845 between Francis Pettygrove, who hailed from a different Portland, in Maine, and Asa Lovejoy, from Boston (the one in Massachusetts) eventually decided the matter. But things might have turned out differently, per Frantisek Bartos, a graduate student at the University of Amsterdam, if people were not such wobbly tossers.

Mr Bartos was interested in an intriguing prediction made by Persi Diaconis, Susan Holmes and Richard Montgomery, a group American mathematicians. In a paper in 2007 the trio analysed the physics of a flipping coin and noticed something intriguing. Besides sending it somersaulting end-over-end, most people tend to impart a slight rotation to a coin when they toss it. That causes the axis about which the coin is flipping to drift while it is in the air, a phenomenon called precession.

The upshot, when the numbers were crunched, was that a coin thrown by a human should exhibit a subtle but persistent bias. Dr Diaconis and his colleagues reckoned there was about a 51% chance that a coin would land the same way up as it had been prior to being thrown. If it was heads-up in the thrower’s hand, in other words, it would be slightly more likely to land heads-up too. Or at least, that was the prediction.

Enter Mr Bartos, and his admirable dedication to empiricism. He rounded up 48 volunteers and convinced them to perform (and film) 350,707 coin tosses, using dozens of different coins, from an Indian two-rupee piece to a Swiss two-franc coin. Sure enough, his data confirmed what the physics had predicted. The coins landed same-side up 50.8% of the time.

The statistics revealed that the coins themselves showed no particular bias. The determining factor was indeed humans’ apparent inability to throw straight. Mr Bartos was not the first person to collect statistics on coin tosses. But he is the first to have done so on a scale large enough to detect the bias. (A previous effort of 40,000 tosses, conducted by two students at the University of California, Berkeley, lacked the statistical power to confirm the theory.)

A 50.8% chance is only very slightly different from perfect fairness. But Mr Bartos points out that it is bigger than the advantage enjoyed by a casino in most varieties of blackjack. And in some situations it may matter. In 2019 Sue Cudilla became mayor of Araceli, a town in the Philippines, on the toss of a coin after the election had been declared a dead heat. Even more importantly, a coin toss can determine who bowls or bats first in cricket. Professional athletes spend thousands of dollars and hours of training in search of marginal gains. Perhaps they should look to the loose change in the umpire’s pocket.

Scientists have published an atlas of the brain

Cataloguing its components may help understand how it works

American and Chinese scientists are decoupling, too

That will be bad for both countries


Like human armies, army ants trail crowds of hangers-on

One insect’s leavings is another’s dinner